Volume 1. No. 6 |
|
|
|
June 21, 2019 |
|
|
Abstract: A
letter to the editor from Richard Woods and Mitzi Waltz |
The strength of autistic expertise and its
implications for autism knowledge production: A response to Damian Milton.
By Richard Woods and Mitzi Waltz
Dear Larry Arnold.
Milton (2017) describes different types of expertise
to be found amongst autism stakeholders and notes that there can be an exchange
of expertise between stakeholders. Traditionally the autistic perspective has
been neglected in autism knowledge production leading to distrust from autistic
persons towards researchers, and contributing to a negative feedback cycle of
increasing tensions between stakeholder groups. This article revisits these
claims in light of the growing case for seeking autistic expertise, including
how ignoring this could fuel stakeholder distrust and rivalry entrenching the
prevailing biases within each stakeholder group (Milton, 2017). Additionally,
autistic academics are increasingly producing scholarship at masters, doctoral
and professional level (Woods et al 2018); reinforcing biases could silence
this important source of autistic voice. We provide suggestions on emancipatory
knowledge production methods to start a debate on how best to remedy this
source of stakeholder conflict. Before, this article explores the strength of
autistic expertise including recent research developments.
The autistic theory of monotropism
can be viewed as the strongest autism theory. It explains both the cognitive
and sensory differences experienced by autistic persons (Chown, 2017; Murray,
Lesser & Lawson, 2005). Monotropism, views
autistic experiences as based around interest creating “attention tunnels”
where the amount of processing resource or attention each person can utilise at any moment is a limited resource. How each
person experiences attention varies forming a continuum, with polytropism at one extreme and monotropism
at the other. Monotropism is
a single, hyper focused attention tunnel compared to polytropism
when an individual has multiple simultaneous slightly aroused or primed
interests, with a low level processing flow constantly connecting them. During
a monotropic state, perception is hyper focused on a
narrow range of subjects which may be broad or deep in themselves,
while outside stimuli are occluded from perception. Sudden interruptions to monotropic states can be highly distressing and
disorientating, proportional to the intensity of monotropic
state and the severity of its ending. Such occurrences explain demand avoidance
behaviour in Pathological Demand Avoidance. It is
argued that the characteristic spiky skills profile is caused by which
interests arouse a monotropic state, while other
skills remain side felt experience (Milton, 2017). Monotropism
(Murray, Lesser & Lawson, 2005) clearly offers much to elucidate traits
associated with autism as compared to the main cognitive theories, conversely
at present it is not widely recognised (Chown, 2017;
Milton, 2017). As many research articles do not rely on autism theory (Chown,
2017), there is also little prevent further exploration of monotropism.
Increasingly evidence supports placing
autistic perspectives centrally within autism studies, as with regards to
specifically supporting autistic critiques of pathologising
discourses and treatment. For instance a recent study reported that almost half
of autistic individuals who received the intervention Applied Behavioural Analysis often experienced Posttraumatic Stress
Disorder, with autistic ABA recipients 86% more likely to experience PTSD
criteria than others with autism (Kupferstein, 2018).
Milton’s double empathy problem (2017) interrogates locating social
communication issues experienced by autistic persons as caused solely by
autistic Theory of Mind deficits. Crucially, wider research lends credence to
autistic views on the harmful nature of pathologising
treatments and the benefit for autistic persons of being in control of
therapeutic directions (Milton, 2017). Inevitably, as more progressive
literature arises, autistic academics and their allies will increasingly lobby
for emancipatory autism knowledge production.
In light of research practices and policies
that increasingly place patient, service user and pupil voice central in other
fields (for example see Shippee et al 2015), we agree that it is unsustainable to ignore the case for
autistic parity with other stakeholders in autism knowledge production.
Further, with each passing year there is increasing momentum for research to be
autistic-led on both epistemic and ethical grounds. A debate is urgently
required regarding how parity would look in practice, the evidence threshold
needed to trigger this paradigm shift, and how “autistic-led” should be
defined. We propose two methods that could be adopted by the autism research
community that may help to solve these stakeholder tensions. Firstly, there
needs to be a debate where autistic scholarship compliments or succeeds
traditional key literature: for instance a case can be made for monotropism being accepted as a key autism theory (Chown,
2017; Milton, 2017; Murray, Lesser & Lawson, 2005), and this should give
rise to further empirical research. We also recommend wider adoption of
inclusive practice, criticality, new lines of inquiry and epistemological
integrity. These traits intersect on many different levels in critical autism
studies (Woods et al, 2018). If adopted,
either of these proposals should acknowledge autistic perspectives, while
decrease inter and intra stakeholder mistrust conflict. Failure to recognise the strength of autistic perspectives,
on the other hand holds back progress and increases the risk of poor quality
research.
Yours sincerely.
Mr Richard Woods.
Dr Mitzi Waltz.
References.
Chown, N. (2017). Understanding
and Evaluating Autism Theory. London, UK: Jessica Kingsley Publishers.
Kupferstein, H. (2018). Evidence of increased PTSD symptoms in autistics
exposed to applied behaviour analysis. Advances in Autism. 4(1): 19-29.
Milton, D. E. M. (2017). A Mismatch of
Salience: Explorations of the nature of autism from theory to practice. Hove,
UK: Pavilion Publishing and Media Ltd.
Murray, D., Lesser, M.,
& Lawson, L. (2005). Attention, monotropism and the diagnostic criteria for autism. Autism,
9: 139–156.
Shippee, N., Domecq Garces, J., Prutsky Lopez, G., Wang, Z., Elraiyah,
T., Nabhan, M.,…Murad, M.
(2015). Patient and service user engagement in research: A systematic review
and synthesized framework. Health Expectations, 18(5): 1151-1166.
Woods, R., Milton, D.,
Arnold, L., & Graby, G. (2018). Redefining Critical Autism Studies: a more inclusive
interpretation. Disability & Society, 33(6): 974-979.
Refbacks
- There are currently no refbacks.

This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License.